Let us consider two explanations

Let us consider two explanations

Mental illness happen as results from the dynamic interplay of biological (genetics, neurobiology), psychological (personality, coping styles), and social risks (relationships, trauma, socioeconomic status). Medication and psychotherapy can help restore this balance.

Mental illness are tests from God that can make us stronger spiritually. One can find the peace through prayer, remembering God, and trusting that difficulties have purpose.

Which one do you personally think more convincing or most useful?

Some people may be convinced that it’s one explanation against another, but for others, the answer can be less straightforward.

Desiderata

Warning

This presentation, however, won’t dig deeper into whether or not these two types of explanations logically or illogically compatible, its truthfulness quality, or accuracy.

Rather, and more interestingly, what does it mean to believe in science and religion, psychologically?

How do people acquire scientific and religious explanations?

Do you still remember how do you first learn that God and oxygen, while both are invisible, exist?

How do people acquire scientific and religious explanations?

  • Plenty of evidence demonstrates that we naive theories about unobservable entities (e.g., God, bacteria, Angels, mental processes, etc.) primarily through testimony (Ma et al. 2024).

  • This means we learn these explanations by combining direct experience (McLoughlin et al. 2023) and cultural input (Davoodi and Clegg 2022).

    • Direct experience: first-hand, observational experience that is independent of others’ testimony.
    • Cultural input: information we obtain from other individuals of our group and community, both verbally or symbolically.
  • Testimony don’t matter if we don’t trust the sources, so trust here is essential for belief acquisition (Hendriks, Kienhues, and Bromme 2016).

How do people acquire scientific and religious explanations?

When acquiring scientific explanations, cultural input is much less central, rendering individuals more confident with their scientific than religious beliefs (Harris and Corriveau 2021).

Shared Psychological Functions

Not only people develop their scientific and religious understanding in the same manner, science and religion also satisfy similar psychological needs (Rutjens and Preston 2020). Specifically, they fulfill:

  • Need for explanation: to understand why events occur and how things work, seeking causal accounts that make sense of experiences and observations
  • Need for control: to influence outcomes and have agency over one’s environment

Shared Psychological Functions

Not only people develop their scientific and religious understanding in the same manner, science and religion also satisfy similar psychological needs (Rutjens and Preston 2020). Specifically, they fulfill:

  • Need for existential meaning: to find purpose, significance, and coherence in existence

If scientific and religious belief are acquired similarly and fulfill the exact same needs, does it mean they are psychologically competing for our mental resource?

Secularization and Conceptual Change

It’s, of course, natural to assume that religious and scientific explanations are competing for our mental resource.

Literature proposes that the competition occurs not only at individual, but also at institutional level.

  • Secularization hypothesis: scholars (e.g., Freud, Durkheim) have long predicted that religion would lose its relevance with the rapid emergence of industrialization (Inglehart and Norris 2007).
  • Conceptual change hypothesis: as children start to learn science, their naive theories, including religious beliefs, are accommodated (in Piagetian term), or completely replaced by scientific beliefs (Posner et al. 1982).

Secularization and Conceptual Change

Warning

If these two hypotheses are correct, then we should observe that religious institutions cease to exist in the face of scientific progress and individuals completely abandon their religious beliefs when they receive scientific training.

We do observe these patterns, at least partially.

  • There is a declining trend of engagement in religious activities - more strongly in industrialized secular countries (Inglehart and Norris 2007).
  • Evidence shows that people tend to ascribe incompatibility between science and religion when they are entering higher education (Scheitle and Corcoran 2021; Shipman et al. 2002).
  • Plenty large-scale research demonstrates that reflective and critical thinking, cognitive process fundamental in fostering scientific beliefs, are correlated with God disbelief (Ghasemi et al. 2025).

Secularization and Conceptual Change

BUT…

Religion remains a powerful social force that guides individuals’ decision. Many people deem believe in God is essentially needed for one to be moral (Tamir, Connaughton, and Salazar 2020).

While scientists are less likely to be religious than the general population (Ecklund 2010), many distinguished scientists are also faithful believers.

People at different ages and different cultures are found to engage in both scientific and religious explanations to explain various phenomena, even with or without scientific understanding. In psychology, we called this phenomenon explanatory coexistence.

Explanatory Coexistence

Adults and children in many different cultures are found to be more likely to engage in both scientific and religious/supernatural explanation rather than with only scientific or religious explanations alone…

…most prominently to explain death, illness, and origins (e.g., Why do people die? Why do people get sick? Where do we come from?) (Legare et al. 2012; Legare and Gelman 2008).

This coexistence is observed even when scientific literacy is present and salient (Haimila, Metsähinen, and Sevalnev 2024).

According to explanatory coexistence hypothesis, individuals use scientific and religious explanations by applying different ‘frameworks’ with varying levels of integration.

Explanatory Coexistence

These frameworks are:

  • Target-dependent thinking: using scientific and religious explanations for addressing different aspects of a phenomenon
  • Synthetic thinking: partially integrate scientific and religious explanations by loosely combining them
  • Integrative thinking: fully integrate scientific and religious explanation into a unified, coherent explanation

Deficient or adaptive?

Warning

If scientific and religious beliefs are competing for our mental resources, why do they coexist as observed by explanatory coexistence literature?

Some people argue that the pervasiveness of religious beliefs in the face of scientific evidence is indicative of deficient reasoning due to incomplete learning, but more and more evidence shows that this is due to science and religion having differential utility.

Functional Differentiation

The pervasiveness of religious beliefs indicate that they serve different roles, thus people apply different rules when processing scientific and religious explanations.

Science for epistemic purposes (‘to know’), religion for non-epistemic purposes (e.g., moral, social, power, status) (Davoodi and Lombrozo 2022).

Factual (scientific) beliefs are involuntarily formed in response to evidence and evaluated based on truth-bearing quality, while religious credence is voluntarily chosen, context-dependent, and evaluated based on identity and normative values rather than evidence (Van Leeuwen 2014).

Religious explanations have a lower evidentiary standard (Lobato et al. 2020).

Some Early Empirical Evidence (Zein & Gollwitzer, 2025)

We did an experiment involving 719 German participants, where participants were presented with scientific and religious explanations of three incidents: flood, climate crisis, and war.

Participants then indicated to what extent they thought that each of these explanations useful.

We were testing a motivated reasoning hypothesis: religious participants would rated religious (vs. scientific) explanations as having higher utility, while non-religious people would indicate that scientific (vs. religious) explanation as being more useful.

Some Early Empirical Evidence (Zein & Gollwitzer, 2025)

“Throughout history, human civilization has faced enormous collective adversity, including violent war. Millions of people have died, been displaced, and others have been indirectly affected by the devastating effects of violent war. “Why are there so many violent wars in the world?” you may ask. Below are plausible explanation(s) for the causes of violent war.”

Research on intergroup relations shows that collective memories of past conflicts, injustices, or humiliations can fuel war. When societies or groups focus on these historical grievances, they may seek revenge or retribution, perpetuating cycles of violence.

Although God teaches peace and reconciliation, He also allows wars as a means of restoring justice. People must be punished for some wrongdoings and are therefore allowed to punish each other. Therefore, war is something that is ultimately willed by God.

Some Early Empirical Evidence (Zein & Gollwitzer, 2025)

Instead, this what we found

Some Early Empirical Evidence (Zein & Gollwitzer, 2025)

Instead, this what we found

What does this implicate?

  • The motivated reasoning effect is asymmetry - only religious explanations are affected by religiosity.
  • Scientific explanations are virtually intact - religiosity didn’t matter when participants were asked to rate their perceived utility.
  • Scientific explanations enjoy a kind of universal acceptance, while religious explanations don’t.
  • This explains why scientific and religious explanations coexist.

Summary

  • People can engage meaningfully in both scientific and religious explanations, because..
    • They are useful for different purposes.
    • People apply different rules to scientific and religious beliefs when processing these two types of explanations.
    • Religious/supernatural beliefs, therefore, cannot be completely replaced by and don’t hinder scientific understanding.

Summary

References

Altenmüller, Marlene Sophie, Lorenz Kampschulte, Laura Verbeek, and Mario Gollwitzer. 2023. “Science Communication Gets Personal: Ambivalent Effects of Self-Disclosure in Science Communication on Trust in Science.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000489.
Altenmüller, Marlene Sophie, Leonie Lucia Lange, and Mario Gollwitzer. 2021. “When Research Is Me-Search: How Researchers’ Motivation to Pursue a Topic Affects Laypeople’s Trust in Science.” PLOS ONE 16 (7): e0253911. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253911.
Davoodi, Telli, and Jennifer M. Clegg. 2022. “When Is Cultural Input Central? The Development of Ontological Beliefs about Religious and Scientific Unobservables.” Child Development Perspectives 16 (1): 34–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12435.
Davoodi, Telli, and Tania Lombrozo. 2022. “Explaining the Existential: Scientific and Religious Explanations Play Different Functional Roles.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 151 (5): 1199. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001129.
Ecklund, Elaine Howard. 2010. Science Vs. Religion: What Scientists Really Think.
Ghasemi, Omid, Onurcan Yilmaz, Ozan Isler, Jenny Terry, and Robert M. Ross. 2025. “Reflective Thinking Predicts Disbelief in God Across 19 Countries.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, April. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-025-02691-9.
Haimila, Roosa, Hanne Metsähinen, and Mark Sevalnev. 2024. “Supernatural Belief in ‘Scientific’ Worldviews?: Investigating Science-oriented Finns’ Explanations for Origins, Death and Suffering.” Journal of Cognition and Culture 24 (1-2): 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340181.
Harris, Paul L, and Kathleen H Corriveau. 2021. “Beliefs of Children and Adults in Religious and Scientific Phenomena.” Current Opinion in Psychology, Religion, 40 (August): 20–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.08.003.
Hendriks, Friederike, Dorothe Kienhues, and Rainer Bromme. 2016. “Trust in Science and the Science of Trust.” In, 143–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28059-2_8.
Inglehart, Ronald, and Pippa Norris. 2007. “Why Didn’t Religion Disappear? Re-Examining the Secularization Thesis.” In Conflicts and Tensions, by Helmut Anheier and Yudhishthir Isar, 253–57. 1 Oliver’s Yard, 55 City Road, London EC1Y 1SP United Kingdom: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446214671.n24.
Legare, Cristine H., E. Margaret Evans, Karl S. Rosengren, and Paul L. Harris. 2012. “The Coexistence of Natural and Supernatural Explanations Across Cultures and Development.” Child Development 83 (3): 779–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01743.x.
Legare, Cristine H., and Susan A. Gelman. 2008. “Bewitchment, biology, or both: the co-existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development.” Cognitive Science 32 (4): 607–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802066766.
Lobato, Emilio J. C., Shadab Tabatabaeian, Morgan Fleming, Sven Sulzmann, and Colin Holbrook. 2020. “Religiosity Predicts Evidentiary Standards.” Social Psychological and Personality Science 11 (4): 546–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619869613.
Ma, Shaocong, Ayse Payir, Niamh McLoughlin, and Paul L. Harris. 2024. “Scientific and Religious Beliefs Are Primarily Shaped by Testimony.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, May. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2024.04.014.
McLoughlin, Niamh, Yixin Kelly Cui, Telli Davoodi, Ayse Payir, Jennifer M. Clegg, Paul L. Harris, and Kathleen H. Corriveau. 2023. “Expressions of Uncertainty in Invisible Scientific and Religious Phenomena During Naturalistic Conversation.” Cognition 237 (August): 105474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105474.
Posner, George J., Kenneth A. Strike, Peter W. Hewson, and William A. Gertzog. 1982. “Accomodation of A Scientific Conception: Toward a Theory of Conceptual Change.” Science Education 66 (2): 211–27. https://staff.science.uva.nl/e.joling/vakdidactiek/documenten/posner.pdf.
Rutjens, Bastiaan T., and Jesse L. Preston. 2020. “Chapter 26 - Science and Religion: A Rocky Relationship Shaped by Shared Psychological Functions.” In The Science of Religion, Spirituality, and Existentialism, edited by Kenneth E. Vail and Clay Routledge, 373–85. Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-817204-9.00027-5.
Scheitle, Christopher P., and Katie E. Corcoran. 2021. “Endorsement of Religion–Science Conflict as an Expression of Group Solidarity Among Graduate Students in the Sciences.” Sociology of Religion 83 (1): 79–101. https://doi.org/10.1093/socrel/srab003.
Shipman, Harry L., Nancy W. Brickhouse, Zoubeida Dagher, and William J. Letts. 2002. “Changes in Student Views of Religion and Science in a College Astronomy Course.” Science Education 86 (4): 526–47. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.10029.
Tamir, Christine, Aidan Connaughton, and A.M. Salazar. 2020. “Is Belief in God Necessary for Good Values? Global Survey on Religion and Morality.” The Global God Divide. Washington DC: Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2020/07/20/the-global-god-divide/.
Van Leeuwen, Neil. 2014. “Religious Credence Is Not Factual Belief.” Cognition 133 (3): 698–715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.015.